Guest post by Blake Alexander Simmons, Student Blogging Contest Series 2019
The morning of
April 16, I awoke to several messages from European and American friends
lamenting the fire engulfing the Notre-Dame cathedral in Paris. Within the next
48 hours, I witnessed one of the most rapid evolutions in public outcry that I
had ever seen. What began as world-wide sorrow and empathy for the destruction
of an international landmark, turned into hope and comfort as billionaires
pledged hundreds of millions of euros to restore the cathedral. This eventually
led to global outrage at this outpouring of financial support. Suddenly, social
media was ablaze with criticism over the amount of money being donated to
restore this UNESCO World Heritage site. My head hung as I watched as friends,
colleagues, and influential scientists in the conservation community expressed
their frustration that these billionaires were not similarly donating to other
environmental causes, like the restoration of the Great Barrier Reef. They
couldn’t believe that a church composed of wood, stone, and glass would be so
much more important than a healthy, functioning environment. That is, they
couldn’t understand why these billionaires didn’t hold the same values as them,
and were subsequently villainised for their actions.
When we
observe a particular behavior that doesn’t align with our own values, beliefs,
or agendas, it can be easy to vilify that behavior—even when it is altruistic
in nature. The criticisms surrounding the financial support for the Notre-Dame
highlight the importance that agendas play in supporting or denigrating
observations and evidence in the scientific community. In my experience, this
is especially true in the conservation disciplines. Agendas are important; they
guide the goals and objectives of institutions, organisations, governments, and
the individuals they are composed of. But they can also be problematic when
they are grounded in unwavering beliefs or perceptions that hinder the
important reflective and adaptive capacities of these agendas.
Like
Notre-Dame, when confronted with behaviors or evidence that contrast with our
own agendas, we must not dismiss them as ‘right’ or ‘wrong,’ but reflect on how
they align or can be aligned with our own agendas. In this post, I will discuss
my own personal experiences during my doctoral candidacy related to this
important issue, outlining the struggles I’ve experienced trying to promote
sustainable environmental behavior interventions when the evidence does not
align with fixed environmental agendas. I’ll highlight the importance of
objective conservation research, and I’ll challenge those translating evidence
into practice and policy to be adaptable and receptive to emerging evidence—even
if it conflicts with pre-determined beliefs.
A modern deforestation context
The state of
Queensland, Australia is facing globally-significant rates of deforestation,
despite a recent history of regulatory intervention to control what landholders
can clear on their properties. Extensive land clearing throughout the state
began during the height of development in Queensland (ca. 1940–1980), as the government incentivised landholders to clear
as much native vegetation as possible in order to grow the emerging economy.
This financial incentive was prevalent throughout most of the 20th
century, until public recognition of the importance of dwindling habitats began
to grow. This eventually led to the enactment of the Vegetation Management Act (Qld) 1999, which remains the primary
mechanism for regulating the clearing of remnant (i.e. old-growth) vegetation
on private lands in the state. Fraught with controversy, this
command-and-control policy instrument has undergone considerable evolution over
the last 20 years as restrictions tighten, loosen, and tighten again. Today, conservation
groups and agricultural groups all agree vegetation management policy needs a
change; the direction of that change, however, is hotly debated. As farmers and
agricultural lobbyists fight to reduce regulatory control, conservationists and
environmental NGOs continue to push for stronger policies to reduce
deforestation.
Trends in tree clearing across Queensland, Australia against a dynamic
timeline of vegetation management policy. Source: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0264837717308608
|
Given this
ongoing debate, the research for my PhD investigated how deforestation
behaviors have changed amidst these frequent policy changes, how effective
regulatory intervention has been at reducing deforestation, and what
biophysical, political, and social factors may be driving landholders’ clearing
decision-making. As you might imagine, the subject of my research has (at
first) been met with great intrigue and anticipation by conservation interest
groups; unfortunately, the subsequent results are often met with great
disappointment. Why? The evidence does not support the current conservation
agenda, which pushes for stronger command-and-control tactics to create
pro-environmental behavior change: landholders in historical clearing hotspots
have not been responsive to policy restrictions, the Act has only been marginally effective at reducing remnant deforestation, and frequent periods of policy uncertainty have resulted in spikes in pre-emptive clearing,
which have reduced the positive impacts of regulatory intervention.
Not so simple: the importance of social science in conservation
As any social
scientist will tell you, creating desired behavior change is difficult. The
mind is a complex place, where unobservable factors, biases, perceptions, and
experiences shape every day decision-making. Policy instruments, like direct regulation,
do not necessarily result in simple cause-effect behaviour outcomes. The
importance of many social science fields, like behavioral economics and social
psychology, cannot be emphasized enough when we talk about conservation. In the
context of deforestation in Queensland, a consideration of the social factors
driving decision-making is imperative for creating positive behavior change in
the future.
Why are some landholders not responding as expected to
regulatory restrictions? Why has policy intervention had marginal success? How
can we prevent perverse outcomes from intervention? While I don’t expect to have all of the answers to
these questions, current evidence utilizing social science has revealed some
important insights. For example, my most recent projects investigate the
different typologies of farmers, drivers of clearing intentions, and
differential preferences for financial vs. non-financial incentives for bush
preservation (all currently in review). The results emphasize a number of
important psychosocial characteristics, like social norms, the perceived threat
of regulation, and self-identity. Without addressing these underlying factors,
future top-down approaches to curbing deforestation may continue to have limited
effectiveness. Instead, more bottom-up approaches should be implemented, which
can focus on building trust and support networks, improving the relationships
between landholders and extension officers, and promoting pro-environmental
norms and stewardship identities within communities.
Perhaps
naively, I never would have expected such recommendations would be so
unfavourable to so many in the conservation community. Throughout my
candidature, I have consistently had to defend these interdisciplinary approaches
and their alternative solutions. For many conservation social scientists, this
is nothing new. Quantitative natural scientists have a long history of
dismissing social science approaches. From ‘human
behaviour is not nearly as complicated as ecological interactions’ to ‘the only thing that will make people change
is money or the threat of imprisonment’—I have heard it all. And as someone
trained as a quantitative natural scientist, I get it. Traditionally, our
knowledge of the human dimensions of conservation has been limited, and true
interdisciplinarity is still not entirely supported in many institutions. While
I have been fortunate enough to work in a team where interdisciplinarity is not
only encouraged, it is understood as a necessity for creating positive
environmental change, I am constantly confronted with situations where robust
evidence is dismissed or refuted based upon pre-established beliefs,
unconscious biases, and competing agendas.
Bias, dissonance, and conflicts of interest
There are a
number of cognitive biases that may influence our decision-making on a daily
basis, and renowned scientists and influential organisations are not immune to
their effects. Some common examples include confirmation bias (only listening
to information that confirms one’s pre-established beliefs), conservatism bias
(favouring prior evidence to new evidence), and blind-spot bias (recognition of
others’ biases, but a lack of consideration of one’s own biases). While most
conservationists will be able to identify these biases in the environmental
opposition (e.g. confirmation bias in climate change deniers), there is a lack
of reflection on how frequently conservationists are subject to these same
biases.
Understandably,
the mind will always be prone to bias, as we frequently seek to minimise
cognitive dissonance—the mental conflict we experience when confronted with
information or behaviours that contradict our values or beliefs. People will
deal with this dissonance in different ways, and individuals vary substantially
in how they respond to criciticism, their ability to adapt, and their
willingness to engage with others. The conservation community, in particular,
represents a broad spectrum of actors that will differentially respond to
dissonance. But as conservation scientists, we have a duty to remain objective
and independent in our investigations. This can be exceptionally difficult if
we are unaware of how our values, agendas, and experiences influence our
research questions, interpretations, and recommendations.
Is objectivity
even possible in conservation? Within the Queensland deforestation space, I
often find myself as the lone foreigner in a room full of Australians. Most
stakeholders involved in the land clearing debate have developed a set of
attitudes based on a lifetime of past experiences in this Australian context.
Do they have the capacity to be objective and independent from any
preconceptions? During my initial investigations into the social dimensions of
tree clearing in Queensland, I held a focus group with landholders, government
officials, and industry stakeholders. One government official identified this
lack of objectivity as the root of the problem:
“So you say, ‘We need
a fact-based approach. We need rigorous research.’ That research has been done
for a long time. It never got out because it was shut out by the political
forces that don’t want to see it. What’s got to change? You’ve got to argue
these things on a rational approach, and you cannot do that when both sides are
in an emotional state of disrepair. It’s not going to go anywhere… [the debate]
has been on now for 25 years, and it’s been irrational on all sides at times.”
My
contribution to this realm, in contrast, is very different; I have no positive
or negative experiences with landholders or politicians, no political
affiliation in Australia, no significant stake in the outcome. My job is to
assess previous interventions and their effects on behaviour and provide
recommendations that will promote the most sustainable environmental future—all
as an outsider looking in. So yes, some level of objectivity is possible in
conservation, and conservationists must be receptive to objective criticism and
reflection.
About the author
Blake Alexander Simmons is a PhD candidate at The
University of Queensland and visiting fellow at Queensland University of
Technology. His research interests are transdisciplinary, with a particular
focus on social-ecological systems and the biophysical, political, and cultural
dimensions influencing environmental behaviors.
No comments:
Post a Comment